K1605_Fitzgerald_60_554x350

 

Choosing texts for early-grade students is critical if educators hope to reach the Common Core goal of improving reading skills. 

 

The text-complexity standard in the Common Core was created with the goal of all students being college- and career-ready at the end of 12th grade. The Common Core authors argued that college and workplace texts are significantly more complex than those in high school, that higher-performing college students are differentiated from lower-performing peers in their ability to answer questions associated with complex text, and that the text-complexity gap between high school and college/workplace must be closed. To close the gap, all students throughout schooling should read more complex texts than they currently do. 

Debate about the text-complexity standard is heated (Hiebert, 2012; Shanahan, 2011; Gamson, Lu, & Eckert, 2013). Some of the fuss is about a text-complexity staircase presented in the standards — a staircase that provides “grade-by-grade specifications (text-level bands) for increasing text complexity in successive years of schooling” (NGA Center for Best Practices & CCSSO, 2010, Appendix A, p. 4). Raising the text-complexity bar for beginning readers is especially controversial because the Core’s 2nd/3rd-grade step ends at 820 Lexiles (L) — about one grade level higher than previous recommendations (Williamson, Fitzgerald, & Stenner, 2014). Historically, while many students have achieved a reading level at or above 820L by the end of 3rd grade, struggling readers have attained, on average, only about 400L by the end of 3rd grade (Williamson et al., 2014). For many children, making up the nearly 400L difference from kindergarten through 3rd grade may require Herculean effort (Williamson et al., 2014).  

Still more fuss is about how early-grades teachers can evaluate texts to know which ones are more or less complex for their students (Hiebert, 2013). The Common Core provides four qualitative indicators: 

  • Levels of meaning or purpose;
  • Structure;
  • Language conventionality and clarity; and
  • Knowledge demands (NGA & CCSSO, 2010).

Yet educators question the extent to which the four indicators are applicable to early-grades texts (Hiebert, 2013). While even the youngest of students are expected to read more complex texts than in the past, the standards are nearly silent on text-complexity factors for early-grades texts.  

Still, the text-complexity standard is a standard, and early-grades teachers in states that adhere to the Common Core are bound by policy to shepherd all students to reach the standards’ goals. As educators attempt to support young children to read increasingly complex texts, they need a firm understanding of what makes beginning-reading texts more or less complex.  

What’s a teacher to do? 

We recently completed a text-complexity study (Fitzgerald et al., 2015) that explores what makes early-grades texts complex. We examined 350 digitized books selected to represent a wide range of kindergarten through 2nd-grade texts. Text-complexity levels for the books were determined using a scale created by combining teachers’ judgment of text complexity and student reading. We identified 22 text characteristics that could be possible contributors to text complexity and created many computerized operations to measure the text characteristics in different ways. We then analyzed the digital texts using the computerized text-characteristic operations to determine which characteristics mattered most in relation to the assigned text-complexity levels. We learned a lot about early-grades text complexity — a lot that can help teachers. 

Early-grades texts are special 

Before teachers even look at the complexity in texts, they need the knowledge and awareness that early-grades texts differ from upper-grades texts in that they are designed specifically to facilitate young students’ progress. Think about what beginning readers are mainly working on: cracking the code. Making meaning with texts is always the focus, but young children especially need to develop the ability to hear sounds in words, develop sight words, and acquire word recognition strategies (Fitzgerald & Shanahan, 2000).  

What’s in a text is important to children’s reading growth because the presence of certain text features can actually facilitate the development of code cracking (Compton, Appleton, & Hosp, 2004), and it’s not generally as clear-cut as easing back on word meaning difficulty and/or using shorter sentences to bring a text down to the beginning-reader level. Lots of word repetition in texts reinforces sight-word learning and development of the sounds associated with spelling patterns (Vadasy, Sanders, & Peyton, 2005). Rhyming words advance the ability to hear sounds in words, a critical factor in learning to read (Adams, 1990). Words that are familiar in meaning in oral language reduce challenges to meaning creation while reading, permitting more attention to word recognition (Muter et al., 2004). Repeated refrains or phrases also reinforce sight word development as well as scaffolding development of a variety of word recognition strategies, such as using context to make guesses at unknown words (Ehri & McCormick, 1998). Moreover, texts that combine several types of text-characteristic support may exponentially scaffold and boost children’s early code-learning development. 

Using text characteristics as indicators  

With the understanding that early grades are special because their construction can springboard children into reading, teachers can then begin to examine texts to match “just right” complexity levels to particular children. They can think about four main groups of text characteristics. After examining the 350 books in our study, we found nine text characteristics that could be aggregated into four constellations:  

  • Word structure demand (decoding demand of words and the number of syllables in words);
  • Word meaning demand (age at which word meanings are acquired, word abstractness, and word rareness);
  • Sentence complexity; and
  • Discourse-level characteristics (diversity of phrases across sentences, text density/information load, and how compressible the information in the text was or wasn’t).

The essence of the discourse-level characteristics is that they signify the degree to which redundancy, repetition, or patterning occurs in a text. An additional key finding was that interplay among text characteristics was important for explaining the complexity of some books.  

To better understand the text characteristics and the four constellations, check out the book Funny Faces and Funny Places (Modern Curriculum Press, 1996b) in Figure 1. The Lexile level of the text is 85, a low text-complexity level. The bottom graph shows how the book ranks against all 350 books in each of the nine text characteristics. So among all 350 of our K-2 books, the decoding demand of words in Funny Faces and Funny Places is somewhat elevated. There are lots of multisyllable words, word meanings are fairly easy for early-grade children, and sentences aren’t very complex. On the other hand, the yellow bars suggest a considerable amount of repetition and redundancy. (Lower bars for all the characteristics, including discourse-level characteristics, mean “easier” texts. So the low bar for discourse-level means lots of repetition/redundancy/patterning). The top graph shows similar information but with four composites to represent the constellations of characteristics. The considerable redundancy in Funny Faces and Funny Places trades off the relatively high-level decoding demand of the text to bring down the Lexile level. 

Teachers who want to select the “just right” text-complexity levels for students don’t have to have graphs for books like the ones in Figure 1. The graph just shows the importance of the characteristics and how they can interplay with one another. Teachers can read part of a book and think about the broad constellations of word decodability and word meanings in relation to student ages, along with syntactic complexity and the extent of repetition and redundancy. Of course, the four broad constellations don’t cover everything the teacher should think about. Whether students have sufficient background knowledge for the text, and whether students are English-language learners are among additional important factors.  

K1605_Fitzgerald_60_tbl1

The four constellations of characteristics make good sense when considering young children’s needs for cracking the code. The specialness of early-grades texts is again apparent in that the four clusters are somewhat different from the four qualitative indicators noted in the Common Core. There is overlap between word meanings and the Common Core’s “meaning/purpose” indicator, and redundancy/patterning could reference an aspect of the Common Core qualitative dimension, “structure.” However, in the early grades, it is important to understand that a special aspect of structure — patterning and repetition — and word decoding demand are additional critical features.  

It’s a balancing act 

Teachers who are evaluating early-grade text complexity should consider how different text characteristics can balance one another. We’ve mentioned that for some texts, whether a text is more or less complex can depend more on the trade-off between selected text characteristics than on any isolated text characteristic.  

Figure 2 shows another relatively low text-complexity book (37L) where the trade-off also matters. Notice that in One Bee Got on the Bus (Modern Curriculum Press, 1996a), the somewhat elevated word-meaning demand is counterbalanced by the large amount of discourse level repetition/patterning/redundancy (the low yellow bars represent a lot of repetition) and low decoding demand. Often, the presence of repetition, redundancy, and patterning can ease reading progress for children when texts have more challenging word structures and/or word meanings because that patterning can scaffold readers’ word recognition strategies.  

Rather than relying entirely on a quantitative indicator of text-complexity level or on individual text characteristics, teachers should think about how characteristics can modulate and balance each other to affect the demand on linguistic knowledge required of the reader. In light of evidence that today’s reading programs tend to have difficult vocabulary, teachers might particularly observe degrees of repetition and patterning because no patterning or relatively little patterning may couple with the difficult vocabulary to result in relatively high challenge to students’ comprehension.  

K1605_Fitzgerald_60_tbl2

Why text is challenging 

Another finding from our research was that patterns across text characteristics in the K-2 range were more varied in lower text-complexity books than in higher text-complexity books. Texts that fell within a low quantitative range — 0L to 200L, for example — showed more differences in composite patterns than texts that fell within higher quantitative range (e.g., 300L to 500L). We can see in Figures 1 and 2 how different the text-characteristic patterns can be at low-complexity levels. On the other hand, Lucy’s Magic Wand (Shaskan, 2008) at 380L has a pattern that’s typical in higher text-complexity levels: Word structure demand is relatively high, word meaning demand is relatively high, and repetition/redundancy/patterning is relatively low.  

Knowing the quantitative level of an early-grades text gives a teacher an entry point into considering the overall complexity level of the text. However, taking a few additional moments to evaluate which text characteristics might account for that complexity level can help a teacher decide whether to give that text to particular students for a particular purpose. For instance, think about label books — books that have a lot of pictures with a word label beside the picture, like, “zoo,” “elephant,” “giraffe,” “bear.” Label books in our study generally received relatively low quantitative levels. Why? In those books, the word structure or decoding demand was often quite high. But think about other text characteristics. Label books often have words that are highly familiar to young children, so word-meaning demand is low. Syntax demand is low because there is literally no syntactic complexity. However, there is no repetition that helps children to figure out words in the way that couplets or triplets do. And of course, children can simply say the name of the picture without reading the word at all.  

So how could a teacher use her knowledge of the text characteristics to decide whether a book is a just-right book for a young student? Let’s imagine she wants a group of children to learn that a spoken word matches to a printed word in a text — a significant early learning-to-read phase called one-to-one matching. She might use a label book for that purpose. Or if she has a group of English-language learners and she wants students to expand their English oral vocabulary, she might use a label book. But if she has a group of typically developing children, and she wants them to learn that a simple consonant-vowel-consonant pattern generally has a short vowel sound — one of the first orthographic patterns children learn to decode — a label book of the type described here isn’t a good choice, even though it’s one of the lower-level books. In short, all texts at a given text-complexity level are not created equal. Knowing the text characteristics in the book is just as important as knowing the text-complexity quantitative level. 

K1605_Fitzgerald_60_tbl3

Rising to the challenge 

The early phases of learning to read are critical because they set the stage for later reading and academic performance and even are associated with later risk for social-emotional and health problems (Masten et al., 2009). Attaining a just-right text challenge level may be more critical in the emergent reading phase than at any other developmental period (Torgesen et al., 2001). In the midst of the text-complexity fuss, early-grades teachers can support students’ reading growth by appreciating the specialness of early-grades texts, evaluating the most essential characteristics that make texts complex, looking for text-characteristic see-saw patterns, and realizing why a text is more or less challenging. Equipped with a firm grasp of what makes early-grades texts more or less complex, teachers can usher young children through texts that are sweet-spot matches to their developmental reading needs.  

References 

Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 

Compton, D.L., Appleton, A.G., & Hosp, M.K. (2004). Exploring the relationship between text-leveling systems and reading accuracy and fluency in 2nd-grade students who are average and poor decoders. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19, 176-184.  

Ehri, L.C. & McCormick, S. (1998). Phases of word learning: Implications for instruction with delayed and disabled readers. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 14, 135-163. 

Fitzgerald, J., Elmore, J., Koons, H., Hiebert, E.H., Bowen, K., Sanford-Moore, E.E., & Stenner, A.J. (2015). Important text characteristics for early-grades text complexity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107 (1), 4-29. 

Fitzgerald, J. & Shanahan, T. (2000). Reading and writing relations and their development. Educational Psychology, 93, 3-22. 

Gamson, D.A., Lu, X., & Eckert, S.A. (2013). Challenging the research base of the Common Core State Standards: A historical reanalysis of text complexity. Educational Researcher, 42, 381-391. 

Hiebert, E.H. (2012). The Common Core’s staircase of text complexity: Getting the size of the first step right. Reading Today, 29 (3), 26-27. 

Hiebert, E.H. (2013). Supporting students’ movement up the staircase of text complexity. The Reading Teacher, 66, 459-468. 

Masten, A.S., Cutuli, J.J., Herbers, J.E., & Reed, M.-G. J. (2009). Resilience in development. In C.R. Snyder & S.J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed.) (pp. 793-796). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 

Modern Curriculum Press. (1996a). One bee got on the bus. Columbus, OH: Author. 

Modern Curriculum Press. (1996b). Funny faces and funny places. Columbus, OH: Author. 

Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M.J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665-681. 

National Governors Association (NGA) Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO). (2010). Common Core State Standards for English language arts & literacy in history/social studies, science, and technical subjects, Appendix A. Washington, DC: Author. www.corestandards.org/the-standards 

Shanahan, T. (2011). Common Core standards: Are we going to lower the fences or teach kids to climb? Reading Today, 29 (1), 20-21. 

Shaskan, T.S. (2008). Lucy’s magic wand. Mankato, MN: Capstone Press. 

Torgesen, J.K., Alexander, A., Wagner, R.K., Rashotte, C., Voeller, K., & Conway, T. (2001). Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 3-58. 

Vadasy, P.F., Sanders, E.A., & Peyton, J.A. (2005). Relative effectiveness of reading practice or word-level instruction in supplemental tutoring: How text matters. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 364-382. 

Williamson, G.L., Fitzgerald, J., & Stenner, A.J. (2014). Student reading growth illuminates the Common Core text-complexity standard: Raising both bars. Elementary School Journal, 115, 230-254. 

 

Citation: Fitzgerald, J., Elmore, J., Hiebert, E.H., Koons, H.H., Bowen, K., Sanford-Moore, E.E., & Stenner, A.J. (2016). Examining text complexity in the early grades. Phi Delta Kappan, 97 (8), 60-65. 

 

ABOUT THE AUTHORS

default profile picture

A. Jackson Stenner

A. JACKSON STENNER is CEO at MetaMetrics and a research professor at The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

default profile picture

Eleanor E. Sanford-Moore

ELEANOR E. SANFORD-MOORE is senior vice president research and development, MetaMetrics.

default profile picture

Elfrieda H. Hiebert

ELFRIEDA H. HIEBERT is president and CEO of TextProject. She is the author of Teaching Words and How They Work: Small Changes for Big Vocabulary Results .

default profile picture

Heather H. Koons

HEATHER H. KOONS is director of research services, MetaMetrics and a clinical assistant professor at The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

default profile picture

Jeff Elmore

JEFF ELMORE is a research engineer at MetaMetrics.

default profile picture

Jill Fitzgerald

JILL FITZGERALD is a research professor and professor emerita at The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and a distinguished research scientist at MetaMetrics, Durham, N.C.

default profile picture

Kimberly Bowen

KIMBERLY BOWEN is senior assessment designer at ACT, Iowa City, Iowa, and a former research associate, MetaMetrics.